dc.contributor | Gutíerrez Domínguez, Germán Antonio | |
dc.contributor | Aprendizaje y Comportamiento Animal | |
dc.creator | Moreno Naranjo, Leonardo | |
dc.date.accessioned | 2020-03-05T12:45:01Z | |
dc.date.available | 2020-03-05T12:45:01Z | |
dc.date.created | 2020-03-05T12:45:01Z | |
dc.date.issued | 2020 | |
dc.identifier | https://repositorio.unal.edu.co/handle/unal/75855 | |
dc.description.abstract | Recent metanalysis suggest that the long-term ideal mate preferences have small predictive validity for indicators of marital satisfaction and the characteristics of the mate that will be end up selecting. This problem has been examined from different theoretical and methodological strategies; however, the classical research has failed to capture the emotional nature of the mate selection process that could involve the preference measurement system (i.e., autoreport), which could affect the predictions. The currently work aims to assess whether emotional states can alter the overall long-term mate preference evaluation, in order to provide elements in the discussion of the predictive power of preferences. Initially, 2 instrument validation processes were implemented, considering that the tools had not been previously adapted for use in the Colombian population. Two experiments were developed with a factorial design 2 (Sex) x 3 (Affective Group). In study 1, 185 university students (45,4% men and 54,6% women) were primed with stimuli related to Love and Sadness, in addition, preference measurements were applied with the IPRS test. In study 2, a new sample of 220 participants (41,4% men and 58,6% women) was primed with Sexual Desire and Terror, additionally a reduced version of the IPRS test was applied. In both studies, sexual differences were found in preference patterns, as has been reported in other research around the world. However, the effect of emotional states on preferences was minimal, compared to other studies examining this phenomenon, which leads to suggest that long-term mate preferences could be resistant to the emotional changes of the evaluators, therefore, it is necessary to continue studying other factors that may affect the predictive validity of preferences. | |
dc.description.abstract | Recientes estudios sugieren que las preferencias ideales de pareja a largo plazo tienen poca validez predictiva respecto a indicadores claves en la selección final: la satisfacción marital y los rasgos de la pareja definitiva. Este problema ha sido examinado desde distintas estrategias teóricas y metodológicas, no obstante, aquellas propuestas fallan en reconocer la naturaleza emocional del proceso de evaluación y selección de parejas, que directamente puede estar afectando las mediciones de preferencias, y por ende la calidad de las predicciones en la selección final. El presente proyecto de investigación pretende evaluar si efectivamente los estados emocionales pueden alterar la evaluación de preferencias de una pareja ideal a largo plazo, con el fin de aportar elementos en la discusión del poder predictivo de las preferencias. Inicialmente, se implementaron 2 procesos de validación de instrumentos, considerando que las herramientas no habían sido adaptadas previamente para su uso en población colombiana. Seguidamente, se desarrollaron dos experimentos con un diseño factorial 2 (Sexo) x 3 (Grupo Afectivo). En el estudio 1, se analizó una muestra de 185 estudiantes universitarios (45,4% hombres y 54,6% mujeres), los cuales fueron expuestos a un procedimiento de inducción emocional con estímulos relacionados con Amor y Tristeza, además de aplicar la prueba de preferencias IPRS. En el estudio 2, una nueva muestra de 220 participantes (41,4% hombres y 58,6% mujeres) fue expuesta a estímulos de Deseo Sexual y Terror, adicionalmente se aplicó una versión reducida de la prueba IPRS. En ambos experimentos se encuentran diferencias sexuales en los patrones de preferencia, tal como se ha reportado en otras investigaciones alrededor del mundo. No obstante, el efecto de los estados emocionales sobre las preferencias es mínimo, a comparación de otros estudios que examinan el fenómeno, lo cual lleva a sugerir que las preferencias de pareja a largo plazo pueden ser resistentes frente al cambio emocional de los evaluadores, por lo tanto, es necesario seguir estudiando otros factores que puedan afectar el problema de predicción. | |
dc.language | spa | |
dc.publisher | Departamento de Psicología | |
dc.publisher | Universidad Nacional de Colombia - Sede Bogotá | |
dc.relation | Al-Shawaf, L., Conroy-Beam, D., Asao, K., & Buss, D. M. (2016). Human emotions: An evolutionary psychological perspective. Emotion Review, 8(2), 173-186. | |
dc.relation | Al-Shawaf, L., Lewis, D. M., & Buss, D. M. (2018). Sex differences in disgust: why are women more easily disgusted than men?. Emotion review, 10(2), 149-160. | |
dc.relation | American Educational Research Association, American Psychological Association, & National Council on Measurement in Education. (1999). Standards for educational and psychological testing (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: American Educational Research Association. | |
dc.relation | Andrade, E. B., & Ariely, D. (2009). The enduring impact of transient emotions on decision making. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 109(1), 1-8. | |
dc.relation | Bateson, M., & Healy, S. D. (2005). Comparative evaluation and its implications for mate choice. Trends in ecology & evolution, 20(12), 659-664. | |
dc.relation | Bechara, A. (2004). The role of emotion in decision-making: evidence from neurological patients with orbitofrontal damage. Brain and cognition, 55(1), 30-40. | |
dc.relation | Bechara, A., y Damasio, A. R. (2005) The somatic marker hypothesis. Games and Economic Behavior, 336-372. | |
dc.relation | Bechara, A., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. R. (2003). Role of the amygdala in decision‐making. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 985(1), 356-369. | |
dc.relation | Bechara, A., Damasio, A. R., Damasio, H., y Anderson, S. W. (1994). Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition, 50(1-3), 7-15. | |
dc.relation | Bianchi-Demicheli, F., Grafton, S. T., & Ortigue, S. (2006). The power of love on the human brain. Social Neuroscience,, 1(2), 90-103. | |
dc.relation | Bless, H., Fiedler, K., & Forgas, J. P. (2006). Mood and the regulation of information processing and behavior. Affect in social thinking and behavior, 6584. | |
dc.relation | Bless, H., & Schwarz, N. (1999). Sufficient and necessary conditions in dual-process models: The case of mood and information processing. In S. Chaiken & Y. Trope (Eds.). Dual-process theories in social psychology (pp. 423–440). New York: Guilford Press. | |
dc.relation | Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1994). Measuring emotion: the self-assessment manikin and the semantic differential. Journal of behavior therapy and experimental psychiatry, 25(1), 49-59. | |
dc.relation | Braun, M. F., & Bryan, A. (2006). Female waist-to-hip and male waist-to-shoulder ratios as determinants of romantic partner desirability. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 23(5), 805-819. | |
dc.relation | Buss, D. M. (1989). Sex differences in human mate preferences: Evolutionary hypotheses tested in 37 cultures. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 12, 1–49. | |
dc.relation | Buss, D. M. (2006a). Strategies of human mating. Psihologijske teme, 15(2), 239-260. | |
dc.relation | Buss, D. M. (2006b). The evolution of love. The new psychology of love, 65-86. | |
dc.relation | Buss, D. M. (2013). Mate Preferences Questionnaire. Measurement Instrument Database for the Social Science. Retrieved from www.midss.ie | |
dc.relation | Buss, D. (2015). Evolutionary psychology: The new science of the mind. Psychology Press. | |
dc.relation | Buss, D. M. (2018). Sexual and emotional infidelity: Evolved gender differences in jealousy prove robust and replicable. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 13(2), 155-160. | |
dc.relation | Buss, D. M., & Shackelford, T. K. (1997). From vigilance to violence: mate retention tactics in married couples. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72(2), 346-361. | |
dc.relation | Buss, D. M., & Shackelford, T. K. (2008). Attractive women want it all: Good genes, economic investment, parenting proclivities, and emotional commitment. Evolutionary Psychology, 6(1). | |
dc.relation | Buss, D. M., Shackelford, T. K., Kirkpatrick, L. A., & Larsen, R. J. (2001). A half century of mate preferences: The cultural evolution of values. Journal of Marriage and Family, 63(2), 491-503. | |
dc.relation | Buss, D. M., & Schmitt, D. P. (1993). Sexual strategies theory: an evolutionary perspective on human mating. Psychological review, 100(2), 204. | |
dc.relation | Buss, D. M., & Schmitt, D. P. (2019). Mate preferences and their behavioral manifestations. Annual Review of Psychology, 70, 77-110. | |
dc.relation | Bynion, T. M., & Feldner, M. T. (2017). Self-assessment manikin. Encyclopedia of personality and individual differences, 1-3. | |
dc.relation | Campbell, L., Chin, K., & Stanton, S. C. (2016). Initial evidence that individuals form new relationships with partners that more closely match their ideal preferences. Collabra: Psychology, 2(1). | |
dc.relation | Carvalho, S., Leite, J., Galdo-Álvarez, S., & Gonçalves, Ó. F. (2012). The emotional movie database (EMDB): A self-report and psychophysiological study. Applied psychophysiology and biofeedback, 37(4), 279-294. | |
dc.relation | Cattell, R. B., & Nesselroade, J. R. (1967). Likeness and completeness theories examined by sixteen personality factor measures on stably and unstably married couples. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 7(4p1), 351. | |
dc.relation | Conroy-Beam, D., & Buss, D. M. (2016a). Mate preferences. In Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science (pp. 1-11). Springer International Publishing. | |
dc.relation | Conroy-Beam, D., & Buss, D. M. (2016b). How are mate preferences linked with actual mate selection? Tests of mate preference integration algorithms using computer simulations and actual mating couples. PloS one, 11(6), e0156078. | |
dc.relation | Conroy-Beam, D., & Buss, D. M. (2016c). Do mate preferences influence actual mating decisions? Evidence from computer simulations and three studies of mated couples. Journal of personality and social psychology, 111(1), 53. | |
dc.relation | Conroy-Beam, D., & Buss, D. M. (2018). Why is age so important in human mating? Evolved age preferences and their influences on multiple mating behaviors. Evolutionary Behavioral Sciences. | |
dc.relation | Clore, G. L., Schwarz, N., & Conway, M. (1994). Affective causes and consequences of social information processing. In R. S. Wyer, Jr. & T. K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition: Basic processes; Applications (pp. 323-417). Hillsdale, NJ, US: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. | |
dc.relation | Cruz, J. E. & Javela, L.G. (2018). Psicología evolucionista, volver a lo natural. En G. Gutiérrez (Ed), Teorías en Psicología. Integración y el futuro de la disciplina (pp. 84-98). Manual Moderno/Ascofapsi/Colpsic. | |
dc.relation | Daly, M., & Perry, G. (2018). Perspectivas evolucionistas sobre el cuidado y el bienestar infantil. En G. Gutiérrez (Ed), Teorías en Psicología. Integración y el futuro de la disciplina (pp. 134-157). Manual Moderno/Ascofapsi/Colpsic. | |
dc.relation | Damasio, A.R., Tranel, D., & Damasio, H. (1991). Somatic markers and the guidance of behavior. In H. Levin, H. Eisenberg, & A. Benton (Eds.), Frontal lobe function and dysfunction (pp. 217-228). New York: Oxford University Press. | |
dc.relation | Darwin, C. (1859). On the origin of species by means of natural selection. London: John Murray. | |
dc.relation | Darwin, C. (1871). The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex (2 vols.). London: John Murray. | |
dc.relation | Dutton, D. G., & Aron, A. P. (1974). Some evidence for heightened sexual attraction under conditions of high anxiety. Journal of personality and social psychology, 30(4), 510. | |
dc.relation | Eastwick, P. W., & Finkel, E. J. (2008). Sex differences in mate preferences revisited: Do people know what they initially desire in a romantic partner?. Journal of personality and social psychology, 94(2), 245. | |
dc.relation | Eastwick, P. W., Finkel, E. J., & Simpson, J. A. (2018). Best practices for testing the predictive validity of ideal partner preference-matching. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 45(2), 167-181 | |
dc.relation | Eastwick, P. W., Luchies, L. B., Finkel, E. J., & Hunt, L. L. (2014). The predictive validity of ideal partner preferences: A review and metaanalysis. Psychological Bulletin, 140, 623–665. | |
dc.relation | Eckland, B. (1968). Theories of mate selection. Eugen. Quart. 15:71-84. | |
dc.relation | Efran, M. G. (1974). The effect of physical appearance on the judgment of guilt, interpersonal attraction, and severity of recommended punishment in a simulated jury task. Journal of Research in Personality, 8(1), 45-54. | |
dc.relation | Ericson, K. M., & Fuster, A. (2014). The endowment effect. Annu. Rev. Econ., 6(1), 555-579. | |
dc.relation | Escobar-Pérez, J., & Cuervo-Martínez, Á. (2008). Validez de contenido y juicio de expertos: una aproximación a su utilización. Avances en medición, 6(1), 27-36. | |
dc.relation | Fisher, R.A. (1915). The evolution of sexual preference. Eugenics Review, (7) 184:192. | |
dc.relation | Fisher, H. (2004). Why we love: Th e nature and chemistry of romantic love. New York: Henry Holt. | |
dc.relation | Fletcher, G. J., & Kerr, P. S. (2010). Through the eyes of love: Reality and illusion in intimate relationships. Psychological bulletin, 136(4), 627. | |
dc.relation | Fletcher, G. J., Simpson, J. A., Campbell, L., & Overall, N. C. (2013). The science of intimate relationships (Vol. 323). Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell. | |
dc.relation | Fletcher, G. J., Simpson, J. A., Campbell, L., & Overall, N. C. (2015). Pair-bonding, romantic love, and evolution: The curious case of Homo sapiens. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10(1), 20-36. | |
dc.relation | Fletcher, G. J., Simpson, J. A., Thomas, G., & Giles, L. (1999). Ideals in intimate relationships. Journal of personality and social psychology, 76(1), 72. | |
dc.relation | Ford, C. S., & Beach, F. A. (1951). Patterns of sexual behavior. New York: Harper and Row. | |
dc.relation | Förster, J., Özelsel, A., & Epstude, K. (2010). How love and lust change people’s perception of relationship partners. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46(2), 237-246. | |
dc.relation | Forgas, J. P. (2011). She just doesn't look like a philosopher…? Affective influences on the halo effect in impression formation. European Journal of Social Psychology, 41(7), 812-817. | |
dc.relation | Forgas, J. P., & Bower, G. H. (1987). Mood effects on person-perception judgments. Journal of personality and social psychology, 53(1), 53. | |
dc.relation | Forgas, J., & Laham, S. (2016). Halo effects. In R. Pohl, Cognitive Illusions: Intriguing Phenomena in Judgement, Thinking and Memory (2nd ed., pp. 276-290). London: Psychology Press. | |
dc.relation | Fuller, R. C., Houle, D., & Travis, J. (2005). Sensory bias as an explanation for the evolution of mate preferences. The American Naturalist, 166(4), 437-446. | |
dc.relation | Gangestad, S. W., & Simpson, J. A. (2000). The evolution of human mating: Trade-offs and strategic pluralism. Behavioral and brain sciences, 23(04), 573-587. | |
dc.relation | Garza, R., Heredia, R. R., & Cieslicka, A. B. (2016). Male and female perception of physical attractiveness: an eye movement study. Evolutionary Psychology, 14(1). | |
dc.relation | Gerlach, T. M., Arslan, R. C., Schultze, T., Reinhard, S. K., & Penke, L. (2019). Predictive validity and adjustment of ideal partner preferences across the transition into romantic relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 116(2), 313. | |
dc.relation | Gilbert, D. T., Gill, M. J., & Wilson, T. D. (2002). The future is now: Temporal correction in affective forecasting. Organizational behavior and human decision processes, 88(1), 430-444. | |
dc.relation | Gillath, O., Mikulincer, M., Birnbaum, G. E., & Shaver, P. R. (2008). When sex primes love: Subliminal sexual priming motivates relationship goal pursuit. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34(8), 1057-1069. | |
dc.relation | Gray, P. B., Garcia, J. R., & Gesselman, A. N. (2018). Age-related patterns in sexual behaviors and attitudes among single US Adults: An evolutionary approach. Evolutionary Behavioral Sciences. | |
dc.relation | Greiling, H., & Buss, D. M. (2000). Women's sexual strategies: The hidden dimension of extra-pair mating. Personality and individual Differences, 28(5), 929-963. | |
dc.relation | Gutiérrez, G. (2010). Selección sexual, comportamiento y aprendizaje. En E. Zerda (Ed.), Bases biológicas del comportamiento animal y humano (pp. 217-240). Bogotá: Universidad Nacional de Colombia. | |
dc.relation | Hall, J. A., & Taylor, S. E. (1976). When love is blind: Maintaining idealized images of one's spouse. Human Relations, 29(8), 751-761. | |
dc.relation | Hamilton, W. D. (1964). The genetical evolution of social behaviour. II. Journal of theoretical biology, 7(1), 17-52. | |
dc.relation | Hamm, A. O., & Vaitl, D. (1993). Affective associations: The conditioning model and the organization of emotions. The structure of emotion, 203-217. | |
dc.relation | Haselton, M. G., Buss, D. M., Oubaid, V., & Angleitner, A. (2005). Sex, lies, and strategic interference: The psychology of deception between the sexes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31(1), 3-23. | |
dc.relation | Haselton, M. G., & Ketelaar, T. (2006). Irrational emotions or emotional wisdom? The evolutionary psychology of affect and social behavior. Affect in social thinking and behavior, 8, 21. | |
dc.relation | Hitczenko, M. (2013). Modeling anchoring effects in sequential Likert scale questions (No. 13-15). Working Papers. | |
dc.relation | Holtgraves, T. (2004). Social desirability and self-reports: Testing models of socially desirable responding. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30(2), 161-172. | |
dc.relation | Huston, T. L., & Houts, R. M. (1998). The psychological infrastructure of courtship and marriage: The role of personality and compatibility in romantic relationships. The developmental course of marital dysfunction, 114-151. | |
dc.relation | Jin, Z., Shiomura, K., & Jiang, L. (2015). Assessing Implicit Mate Preferences among Chinese and Japanese Women by Providing Love, Sex, or Money Cues. Psychological reports, 116(1), 195-206. | |
dc.relation | Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47(2), 363-391. | |
dc.relation | Kaplan, R. M. (1978). Is beauty talent? Sex interaction in the attractiveness halo effect. Sex Roles, 4(2), 195-204. | |
dc.relation | Katsena, L., & Dimdins, G. (2015). An improved method for evaluating ideal standards in self‐perception and mate preferences. Scandinavian journal of psychology, 56(2), 228-235. | |
dc.relation | Kenrick, D. T., & Keefe, R. C. (1992). Age preferences in mates reflect sex differences in human reproductive strategies. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 15(1), 75-91. | |
dc.relation | Ketelaar, T., & Clore, G. L. (1997). Emotion and reason: The proximate effects and ultimate functions of emotions. In Advances in psychology (Vol. 124, pp. 355-396). North-Holland. | |
dc.relation | Kleiman, D. G., & Malcolm, J. R. (1981). The evolution of male parental investment in mammals. In Parental care in mammals (pp. 347-387). Springer, Boston, MA. | |
dc.relation | Kučera, D., & Haviger, J. (2012). Using mood induction procedures in psychological research. Procedia-Social and Behavioral Sciences, 69, 31-40. | |
dc.relation | Lai, V. T., Hagoort, P., & Casasanto, D. (2012). Affective primacy vs. cognitive primacy: Dissolving the debate. Frontiers in psychology, 3, 243. | |
dc.relation | LeDoux, J. E. (1996). The emotional brain: The mysterious underpinnings of emotional life. New York, NY, US: Simon & Schuster. | |
dc.relation | Levinger, G. K., & Snoek, J. D. (1972). Attraction in relationship: A new look at interpersonal attraction. Morristown, NJ: General Learning Press. | |
dc.relation | Li, N. P., & Meltzer, A. L. (2015). The validity of sex-differentiated mate preferences: Reconciling the seemingly conflicting evidence. Evolutionary Behavioral Sciences, 9(2), 89. | |
dc.relation | Li, N. P., Valentine, K. A., & Patel, L. (2011). Mate preferences in the US and Singapore: A cross-cultural test of the mate preference priority model. Personality and Individual Differences, 50(2), 291-294. | |
dc.relation | Loewenstein, G. (2005). Hot-cold empathy gaps and medical decision making. Health Psychology, 24(4S), S49. | |
dc.relation | Lukaszewski, A. W., & Roney, J. R. (2010). Kind toward whom? Mate preferences for personality traits are target specific. Evolution and human behavior, 31(1), 29-38. | |
dc.relation | Matallana, D., Gómez-Restrepo, C., Ramirez, P., Martínez, N. T., & Rondon, M. (2016). El reconocimiento de emociones, la empatía y los juicios morales en la Encuesta Nacional de Salud Mental (ENSM) de 2015 en Colombia. Revista Colombiana de Psiquiatría, 45, 96-104. | |
dc.relation | Matthews, R. N., & Domjan, M. (2011). Selección sexual: competencia de esperma, sistemas de apareamiento y aprendizaje. En G. Gutiérrez & M. R. Papini (Eds.), Darwin y las Ciencias del Comportamiento (pp. 115-136). Bogotá: Universidad Nacional de Colombia/Colegio Colombiano de Psicólogos. | |
dc.relation | Miller, G. F. (1998). A review of sexual selection and human evolution: How mate choice shaped human nature. Handbook of evolutionary psychology: ideas, issues, and applications (pp. 87-130). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. | |
dc.relation | Miller, G. F. (2011). The mating mind: How sexual choice shaped the evolution of human nature. New York: Anchor Books. | |
dc.relation | Miller, G. F., & Todd, P. M. (1998). Mate choice turns cognitive. Trends in cognitive sciences, 2(5), 190-198. | |
dc.relation | Møller, A. P., & Alatalo, R. V. (1999). Good-genes effects in sexual selection. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 266(1414), 85-91. | |
dc.relation | Moreno, L. (2016). Preferencias en rasgos para la selección de parejas a largo plazo. (Tesis de pregrado). Universidad Nacional de Colombia, Bogotá, Colombia. | |
dc.relation | Muñiz, J., & Fonseca-Pedrero, E. (2019). Diez pasos para la construcción de un test. Psicothema, 31(1). | |
dc.relation | Muñiz, J. (2010). Las teorías de los tests: teoría clásica y teoría de respuesta a los ítems. Papeles del Psicólogo: revista del Colegio Oficial de Psicólogos. | |
dc.relation | Murray, S. L., Holmes, J. G., & Griffin, D. W. (1996). The benefits of positive illusions: Idealization and the construction of satisfaction in close relationships. Journal of personality and social psychology, 70(1), 79. | |
dc.relation | Naranjo, A. (2013). El comportamiento proceptivo femenino. (Tesis de maestría). Universidad Nacional de Colombia, Bogotá, Colombia. | |
dc.relation | Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological review, 84(3), 231. | |
dc.relation | Ortega, L. A. & Gutiérrez, G. (2018). Nada en Biología (o Psicología) tiene sentido excepto a la luz de la teoría de la evolución. En G. Gutiérrez (Ed), Teorías en Psicología: Integración y el futuro de la disciplina (pp. 69-83). Manual Moderno/Ascofapsi/Colpsic. | |
dc.relation | Ortiz Rodríguez, J. G. (2019). Papel de la inteligencia social como criterio de selección sexual en mujeres jóvenes. (Tesis de Maestría). Universidad Nacional de Colombia, Bogotá, Colombia. | |
dc.relation | Pianka, E. R. (1970). On r-and K-selection. The American Naturalist, 104(940), 592-597. | |
dc.relation | Pinilla-Palacios, A. (2017). Contagio emocional: Incidencia de los estados emocionales sobre la valoración de expresiones faciales (Maestría en Psicología). Universidad Nacional de Colombia. | |
dc.relation | Philippot, P. (1993). Inducing and assessing differentiated emotion feeling states in the laboratory. Cognition and Emotion, 7(2), 171–193. | |
dc.relation | Provencher, V., & Jacob, R. (2016). Impact of perceived healthiness of food on food choices and intake. Current obesity reports, 5(1), 65-71. | |
dc.relation | Regan, P. C. (1998). What if you can't get what you want? Willingness to compromise ideal mate selection standards as a function of sex, mate value, and relationship context. Personality and social psychology bulletin, 24(12), 1294-1303. | |
dc.relation | Ridley, M. (1993). The red queen: Sex and the evolution of human nature. Penguin UK. | |
dc.relation | Rosenzweig, P. (2007). The Halo Effect...and the Eight Other Business Delusions that Deceive Managers. Riverside: Free Press. | |
dc.relation | Ryan, M.J. (1990): Sexual selection, sensory systems and sensory exploitation. Oxford Surveys in Evolutionary Biology 7: 157–195. | |
dc.relation | Sefcek, J. A., Brumbach, B. H., Vasquez, G., & Miller, G. F. (2007). The evolutionary psychology of human mate choice: How ecology, genes, fertility, and fashion influence mating strategies. Journal of Psychology & Human Sexuality, 18(2-3), 125-182. | |
dc.relation | Shackelford, T. K., Schmitt, D. P., & Buss, D. M. (2005). Universal dimensions of human mate preferences. Personality and Individual Differences, 39(2), 447-458. | |
dc.relation | Scheib, J. E., Gangestad, S. W., & Thornhill, R. (1999). Facial attractiveness, symmetry and cues of good genes. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 266(1431), 1913-1917. | |
dc.relation | Schmitt, D. P. (2014). Evaluating evidence of mate preference adaptations: How do we really know what Homo sapiens sapiens really want? En V. Weekes-Shackelford & T.Shackelford (Eds.), Evolutionary perspectives on human sexual psychology and behavior (pp. 3-39). New York: Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4939-0314-6_1. | |
dc.relation | Schmitt, D. P. (2015) Fundamentals of Human Mating Strategies, in The Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology (ed D. M. Buss), John Wiley & Sons, Inc., Hoboken, NJ, USA. | |
dc.relation | Singh, D. (1993). Adaptive significance of female physical attractiveness: role of waist-to-hip ratio. Journal of personality and social psychology, 65(2), 293-307. | |
dc.relation | Smith, G. T., McCarthy, D. M., & Anderson, K. G. (2000). On the sins of short-form development. Psychological assessment, 12(1), 102. | |
dc.relation | Stanton, J. M., Sinar, E. F., Balzer, W. K., & Smith, P. C. (2002). Issues and strategies for reducing the length of self‐report scales. Personnel Psychology, 55(1), 167-194. | |
dc.relation | Sternberg, R. J. (1986). A triangular theory of love. Psychological review, 93(2), 119. | |
dc.relation | Stoet, G. (2017). PsyToolkit: A novel web-based method for running online questionnaires and reaction-time experiments. Teaching of Psychology, 44(1), 24-31. | |
dc.relation | Sugiyama, L. S. (2015). Physical attractiveness: An adaptationist perspective. The handbook of evolutionary psychology, 1-68. | |
dc.relation | Thomas, A. G., & Stewart-Williams, S. (2018). Mating strategy flexibility in the laboratory: Preferences for long-and short-term mating change in response to evolutionarily relevant variables. Evolution and Human Behavior, 39(1), 82-93. | |
dc.relation | Todd, P. M., Penke, L., Fasolo, B., & Lenton, A. P. (2007). Different cognitive processes underlie human mate choices and mate preferences. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(38), 15011-15016. | |
dc.relation | Tooby, J. & Cosmides, L. (2005). Conceptual foundations of evolutionary psychology. En D. M. Buss (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 5-67): New York: Wiley. | |
dc.relation | Trivers, R. (1972). Parental investment and sexual selection. In B. Campbell (Ed.), Sexual selection and the descent of man 1871-1971, (pp. 136-179). Chicago: Aldine. | |
dc.relation | Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science, 185(4157), 1124-1131. | |
dc.relation | Van den Berghe, P. L. (1983). Human inbreeding avoidance: Culture in nature. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 6(1), 91-102. | |
dc.relation | Verhulst, B., Lodge, M., & Lavine, H. (2010). The attractiveness halo: Why some candidates are perceived more favorably than others. Journal of nonverbal behavior, 34(2), 111-117. | |
dc.relation | Wardle, M. C., Gonzalez, R., Bechara, A., & Martin-Thormeyer, E. M. (2010). Iowa Gambling Task performance and emotional distress interact to predict risky sexual behavior in individuals with dual substance and HIV diagnoses. Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology, 32(10), 1110-1121. | |
dc.relation | Westermann, R., Spies, K., Stahl, G., & Hesse, F. W. (1996). Relative effectiveness and validity of mood induction procedures: A meta‐analysis. European Journal of social psychology, 26(4), 557-580. | |
dc.relation | White, G. L., Fishbein, S., & Rutsein, J. (1981). Passionate love and the misattribution of arousal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41(1), 56. | |
dc.relation | Widaman, K. F., Little, T. D., Preacher, K. J., & Sawalani, G. M. (2011). On creating and using short forms of scales in secondary research. In K. H. Trzesniewski, M. B. Donnellan, & R. E. Lucas (Eds.), Secondary data analysis: An introduction for psychologists (pp. 39-61). Washington, DC US: American Psychological Association. | |
dc.relation | Wieland, A., Durach, C. F., Kembro, J., & Treiblmaier, H. (2017). Statistical and judgmental criteria for scale purification. Supply Chain Management: An International Journal, 22(4), 321-328. | |
dc.relation | Zahavi, A. (1975). Mate selection—a selection for a handicap. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 53(1), 205-214. | |
dc.relation | Zahavi, A., & Zahavi, A. (1999). The handicap principle: A missing piece of Darwin's puzzle. Oxford: Oxford University Press. | |
dc.relation | Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences. American psychologist, 35(2), 151. | |
dc.relation | Zajonc, R.B. (1984). On the primacy of affect. American Psychologist 39, 117–123. | |
dc.relation | Zentner, M., & Eagly, A. H. (2015). A sociocultural framework for understanding partner preferences of women and men: Integration of concepts and evidence. European Review of Social Psychology, 26(1), 328-373. | |
dc.relation | Zeveloff, S. I., & Boyce, M. S. (1980). Parental investment and mating systems in mammals. Evolution, 34(5), 973-982. | |
dc.relation | Moreno, L. (2019). Estados emocionales y sus efectos en la evaluación de parejas a largo plazo. (Tesis de Maestría). Universidad Nacional de Colombia, Bogotá, Colombia. | |
dc.rights | Reconocimiento 4.0 Internacional | |
dc.rights | Acceso abierto | |
dc.rights | http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ | |
dc.rights | info:eu-repo/semantics/openAccess | |
dc.rights | Derechos reservados - Universidad Nacional de Colombia | |
dc.title | Estados emocionales y sus efectos en la evaluación de parejas a largo plazo | |
dc.type | Otro | |