Interaction between domain-specific and domain-general abilities in math´s competence
Interacción entre las capacidades específicas del dominio y las generales en la competencia matemática
dc.creator | Torresi, Sandra | |
dc.date | 2023-05-12T21:54:44Z | |
dc.date | 2023-05-12T21:54:44Z | |
dc.date | 2020-12-07 | |
dc.date.accessioned | 2023-10-03T19:10:26Z | |
dc.date.available | 2023-10-03T19:10:26Z | |
dc.identifier | Torresi, S. (2020). Interaction between domain-specific and domain-general abilities in math´s competence: Interacción entre las capacidades específicas del dominio y las generales en la competencia matemática. Journal of Applied Cognitive Neuroscience, 1(1), 43–51. https://doi.org/10.17981/JACN.1.1.2020.08 | |
dc.identifier | https://hdl.handle.net/11323/10116 | |
dc.identifier | 10.17981/JACN.1.1.2020.08 | |
dc.identifier | 2745-0031 | |
dc.identifier | Corporación Universidad de la Costa | |
dc.identifier | REDICUC - Repositorio CUC | |
dc.identifier | https://repositorio.cuc.edu.co/ | |
dc.identifier.uri | https://repositorioslatinoamericanos.uchile.cl/handle/2250/9168535 | |
dc.description | This article is an approach to some viewpoints about interactions between domain-specific and general cognitive tools involved in the development of mathematical competence. Many studies report positive correlations between the acuity of the numerical approximation system and formal mathematical performance, while another important group of investigations have found no evidence of a direct connection between non-symbolic and symbolic numerical representations. The challenge for future research will be to focus on correlations and possible causalities between non-symbolic and symbolic arithmetic skills and general domain cognitive skills in order to identify stable precursors of mathematical competence. | |
dc.description | Este artículo es una aproximación a diferentes puntos de vista acerca de la interacción entre las habilidades cognitivas de dominio específico y general involucradas en el desarrollo de la competencia matemática. Muchos estudios reportan correlaciones positivas entre la agudeza del sistema de aproximación numérica y el desempeño matemático formal, mientras que otro grupo importante de investigaciones no han hallado evidencias de una conexión directa entre las representaciones numéricas no simbólicas y las simbólicas. El desafío para las futuras investigaciones será focalizar en correlaciones y posibles causalidades entre las habilidades aritméticas no simbólicas, las simbólicas y las habilidades cognitivas de dominio general con el propósito de identificar precursores estables de la competencia matemática. | |
dc.format | 9 páginas | |
dc.format | application/pdf | |
dc.format | application/pdf | |
dc.language | eng | |
dc.publisher | Corporación Universidad de la Costa | |
dc.publisher | Colombia | |
dc.relation | Journal of Applied Cognitive Neuroscience | |
dc.relation | Anobile, G., Cicchini, G. M. & Burr, D. C. (2016). Number as a Primary Perceptual Attribute: A Review. Perception, 45(1-2), 5–31. https://doi.org/10.1177/0301006615602599 | |
dc.relation | Ashkenazi, S., Mark-Zigdon, N. & Henik, A. (2013). Do subitizing deficits in developmental dyscalculia involve pattern recognition weakness? Developmental Science, 16(1), 35–46. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467- 7687.2012.01190.x | |
dc.relation | Allen, K., Higgins, S. & Adams, J. (2019). The relationship between visuospatial working memory and mathematical performance in school-aged children: a systematic review. Educational Psychology Review, 31, 1–23. https://doi. org/10.1007/s10648-019-09470-8 | |
dc.relation | Baddeley, A. D. (2012). Working memory: theories, models, and controversies. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 1–29. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurevpsych-120710-100422 | |
dc.relation | Blankenship, T. L., Keith, K., Calkins, S. D. & Bell, M. A. (2018). Behavioral performance and neural areas associated with memory processes contribute to math and reading achievement in 6-year- old children. Cognitive Development, 45, 141–151, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.07.002 | |
dc.relation | Bonny, J. W. & Lourenco, S. F. (2013). The approximate number system and its relation to early math achievement: Evidence from the preschool years. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114(3), 375–388. https://doi. org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.09.015 | |
dc.relation | Butterworth, B. (2019). Dyscalculia: from Science to Education. New York: Taylor & Francis. | |
dc.relation | Bugden, S. & Ansari, D. (2011). Individual differences in children’s mathematical competence are related to the intentional but not the automatic processing of Arabic numerals. Cognition, 118(1), 32–44. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2010.09.005 | |
dc.relation | Cantlon, J. F. & Brannon, E. M. (2007). Basic math in monkeys and college students. PLoS Biology, 5(12), 2912– 2919. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal. pbio.0050328 | |
dc.relation | Carey, S. (2009). The Origin of Concepts. New York: Oxford Scholarship. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/97801 95367638.001.0001 | |
dc.relation | Chen, Q. & Li, J. (2014). Association between individual differences in nonsymbolic number acuity and math performance: A meta-analysis. Acta Psychologica, 148, 163–172. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.01.016 | |
dc.relation | Chu, F. W., vanMarle, K. & Geary, D. (2015). Early numerical foundations of young children’s mathematical development. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 132, 205–212. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.01.006 | |
dc.relation | Clark, C. A. C., Nelson, J. M., Garza, J., Sheffield, T. D., Wiebe, S. A. & Espy, K. A. (2014). Gaining control: Changing relations between executive control and processing speed and their relevance for mathematics achievement over course of the preschool period. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1–15. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00107 | |
dc.relation | Clearman, J., Klinger, V. & Szücs, D. (2017). Visuospatial and verbal memory in mental arithmetic. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70(9), 1837– 1855. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2016.1209534 | |
dc.relation | Dehaene, S. (2011). The Number Sense. New York: Oxford University Press. | |
dc.relation | Desoete, A., Ceulemans, A., De Weerdt, F. & Pieters, S. (2012). Can we predict mathematical learning disabilities from symbolic and non-symbolic comparison tasks in kindergarten? Findings from a longitudinal study. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 82(1), 64–81. https://doi.org/10.1348/2044-8279.002002 | |
dc.relation | Fanari, R., Meloni, C. & Massidda, D. (2019). Visual and Spatial Working Memory Abilities Predict Early Math Skills: A Longitudinal Study. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 1–9. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02460 | |
dc.relation | Fazio, L. K., Bailey, D. H., Thompson, C. A. & Siegler, R. S. (2014). Relations of different types of numerical magnitude representations to each other and to mathematics achievement. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 123, 53–72. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2014.01.013 | |
dc.relation | Fritz, A., Haase, V. & Räsänen, P. (Eds.) (2019). International Handbook of Mathematical Learning Difficulties. From the Laboratory to the Classroom. Cham: Springer. | |
dc.relation | Geary, D. (2011). Cognitive predictors of achievement growth in mathematics: A five-year longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 47(6), 1539–1552. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025510 | |
dc.relation | Geary, D., Nicholas, A., Li, Y. & Sun, J. (2017). Development change in the influence of domain-general abilities and domain-specific knowledge on mathematics achievement: an eight-year longitudinal study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 109(5), 680–693. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000159 | |
dc.relation | Gilmore, C., Attridge, N., Clayton, S., Cragg, L., Johnson, S., Marlow, N., Simms, V. & Inglis, M. (2013). Individual differences in inhibitory control, not non-verbal number acuity, correlate with mathematics achievement. PLoS One, 8(6), 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0067374 | |
dc.relation | Goffin, C., Vogel, S. E., Slipenkyj, M. & Ansari, D. (2020). A comes before B, like 1 comes before 2. Is the parietal cortex sensitive to ordinal relationships in both numbers and letters? An fMRI-adaptation study. Human Brain Mapping, 41(6), 1591–1610. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.24897 | |
dc.relation | Halberda, J., Ly, R., Wilmer, J. B., Naiman, D. Q. & Germine, L. (2012). Number sense across the lifespan as revealed by a massive Internet-based sample. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(28), 11116–11120. http:// doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1200196109 | |
dc.relation | Hellstrand, H., Korhonen, J., Räsänen, P., Linmmanmaku, K. & Aunio, P. (2020). Reliability and validity evidence of the early numeracy test for identifying children at risk for mathematical learning difficulties. International Journal of Educational Research, 102, 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijer.2020.101580 | |
dc.relation | Honoré, N. & Noël, M. P. (2016). Improving preschoolers’ arithmetic through number magnitude training: The impact of non-symbolic and symbolic training. PloS ONE, 11(11), 1–22. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0166685 | |
dc.relation | Hornung, C., Schiltz, C., Brunner, M. & Martin, R. (2014). Predicting firstgrade mathematics achievement: the contributions of domain-general cognitive abilities, nonverbal number sense, and early number competence. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1–18 https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00272 | |
dc.relation | Izard, V., Sann, C., Spelke E. S. & Streri, A. (2009). Newborn infants perceive abstract numbers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106(25), 10382–1038. https://doi.org/10.1073/ pnas.0812142106 | |
dc.relation | Kuzmina, Y., Tikhomirova, T., Lysenkova, I. & Malykh, S. (2020). Domain-general cognitive functions fully explained growth in nonsymbolic magnitude representation but not in symbolic representation in elementary school children. PLoS ONE, 15(2), 1–23. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0228960 | |
dc.relation | LeFevre, J. A., Fast, L., Skwarchuk, S. L., Smith-Chant, B. L., Bisanz, J., Kamawar, D. & Penner-Wilger, M. (2010). Pathways to mathematics: Longitudinal predictors of performance. Child Development, 81(6), 1753–1767. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01508.x | |
dc.relation | Libertus, M. E., Feigenson, L. & Halberda, J. (2011). Preschool acuity of the approximate number system correlates with school math ability. Developmental Science, 14(6), 1292–300. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01080.x | |
dc.relation | Libertus, M. E., Odic, D., Feigenson, L. & Halberda, J. (2020). Effects of Visual Training of Approximate Number Sense on Auditory Number Sense and School Math Ability. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 1–16. https://doi. org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.02085 | |
dc.relation | Lindskog, M., Winman, A. & Juslin, P. (2014). The association between higher education and approximate number system acuity. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1–10. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00462 | |
dc.relation | Mammarella, I. C., Caviola, S., Giofrè, D. & Szücs, D. (2018). The underlying structure of visuospatial working memory in children with mathematical learning disability. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 36(2), 220–235. https://doi. org/10.1111/bjdp.12202 | |
dc.relation | Matejko, A. A. & Ansari, D. (2016). Trajectories of Symbolic and Non-symbolic Magnitude Processing in the First Year of Formal Schooling. PLOS ONE, 11(3), 1–15. http://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0149863 | |
dc.relation | Mazzocco. M. M., Feigenson, L. & Halberda, J. (2011). Impairs acuity of the approximate number system underlines mathematical learning disability (Dyscalculia). Child Development, 82(4), 1224–1237. https://doi. org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01608.x | |
dc.relation | Nieder, A. (2019). A brain for numbers. The biology of the number instinct. Cambridge: MIT Press. | |
dc.relation | Nieder, A. & Dehaene, S. (2009). Representation of number in the brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 32, 185– 208. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.051508.135550 | |
dc.relation | Nys, J., Ventura, P., Fernandes, T., Querido, L., Leybaert, J. & Content, A. (2013). Does math education modify the approximate number system? A comparison of schooled and unschooled adults. Trends in Neuroscience and Education. 2(1), 13–22. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tine.2013.01.001 | |
dc.relation | Park, J. & Brannon, E. M. (2014). Improving arithmetic performance with number sense training: An investigation of underlying mechanism. Cognition, 133(1), 188–200. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2014.06.011 | |
dc.relation | Piazza, M. (2010). Neurocognitive startup tools for symbolic number representations. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(12), 542–551. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.09.008 | |
dc.relation | Piazza, M. & Izard, V. (2009). How humans count: numerosity and the parietal cortex. The Neuroscientist, 15(3), 261–273. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073858409333073 | |
dc.relation | Purpura, D. J. & Ganley, C. M. (2014). Working memory and language: Skillspecific or domain-general relations to mathematics? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 122, 104–121. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2013.12.009 | |
dc.relation | Rapin, I. (2016). Dyscalculia and the calculating brain. Pediatric Neurology, 61, 11–20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2016.02.007 | |
dc.relation | Revkin, S. K., Piazza, M., Izard, V., Cohen, L. & Dehaene, S. (2008). Does subitizing reflect numerical estimation? Psychological Science, 19(6), 607–614. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02130.x | |
dc.relation | Schneider, M., Beeres, K., Coban, L., Merz, S., Schmidt, S., Stricker, J. & De Smedt, B. (2016). Associations of nonsymbolic and symbolic numerical magnitude processing with mathematical competence: a meta-analysis. Developmental Science, 20(3), 1–16. http://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12372 | |
dc.relation | Siemann, J. & Petermann, F. (2018). Innate or Acquired? Disentangling number sense and early number competencies. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1–13. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00571 | |
dc.relation | Spelke, E. S. (2017). Core Knowledge, Language, and Number. Language Learning and Development, 13(2), 147–170, https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2016.1263572 | |
dc.relation | Szkudlarek, E. & Brannon, E. M. (2017). Does the approximate number system serve as a foundation for symbolic mathematics? Language learning and development: the official journal of the Society for Language Development, 13(2), 171–190. https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2016.1263573 | |
dc.relation | Träff, L. O., Östergren, R. & Skagerlund, K. (2020). Development of early domain-specific and domain-general cognitive precursors of high and low math achievers in grade 6. Child Neuropsychology, 26(8), 1065–1090. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2020.1739259 | |
dc.relation | Vanbinst, K., Ghesquiere, P. & De Smedt, B. (2012). Numerical magnitude representations and individual differences in children’s arithmetic strategy use. Mind, Brain, and Education, 6(3), 129–136. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751- 228X.2012.01148.x | |
dc.relation | Xenidou-Dervou, I., Van Luit, J. E. H., Kroesbergen, E. H., Friso-van den Bos, I., Jonkman, L. M., Van der Schoot, M. & Van Lieshout, E. (2018). Cognitive predictors of children’s development in mathematics achievement: a latent growth modeling approach. Developmental Science, 21(6), 1–51. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12671 | |
dc.relation | Zhou, X., Wei, W., Zhang, Y., Cui, J. & Chen, C. (2015). Visual perception can account for the close relation between numerosity processing and computational fluency. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1–13. https://doi.org/10.3389/ fpsyg.2015.01364 | |
dc.relation | 51 | |
dc.relation | 43 | |
dc.relation | 1 | |
dc.relation | 1 | |
dc.rights | Copyright (c) 2020 Journal of Applied Cognitive Neuroscience | |
dc.rights | Atribución-NoComercial-SinDerivadas 4.0 Internacional (CC BY-NC-ND 4.0) | |
dc.rights | https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/ | |
dc.rights | info:eu-repo/semantics/openAccess | |
dc.rights | http://purl.org/coar/access_right/c_abf2 | |
dc.source | https://revistascientificas.cuc.edu.co/JACN/article/view/3340 | |
dc.subject | Numerical cognition | |
dc.subject | Approximate number system | |
dc.subject | Working memory | |
dc.subject | Cognitive development | |
dc.subject | Cognición numérica | |
dc.subject | Desarrollo cognitivo | |
dc.subject | Sistema de aproximación numérica | |
dc.subject | Memoria de trabajo | |
dc.subject | Precursores | |
dc.title | Interaction between domain-specific and domain-general abilities in math´s competence | |
dc.title | Interacción entre las capacidades específicas del dominio y las generales en la competencia matemática | |
dc.type | Artículo de revista | |
dc.type | http://purl.org/coar/resource_type/c_dcae04bc | |
dc.type | Text | |
dc.type | info:eu-repo/semantics/article | |
dc.type | http://purl.org/redcol/resource_type/ART | |
dc.type | info:eu-repo/semantics/publishedVersion | |
dc.type | http://purl.org/coar/version/c_970fb48d4fbd8a85 |