dc.contributorJiménez Leal, William Alexander
dc.contributorReali Arcos, Florencia
dc.creatorCifuentes Troncoso, Juan Sebastián
dc.date.accessioned2023-08-17T13:37:37Z
dc.date.accessioned2023-09-07T01:21:45Z
dc.date.available2023-08-17T13:37:37Z
dc.date.available2023-09-07T01:21:45Z
dc.date.created2023-08-17T13:37:37Z
dc.date.issued2023-06
dc.identifierhttp://hdl.handle.net/1992/69790
dc.identifierinstname:Universidad de los Andes
dc.identifierreponame:Repositorio Institucional Séneca
dc.identifierrepourl:https://repositorio.uniandes.edu.co/
dc.identifier.urihttps://repositorioslatinoamericanos.uchile.cl/handle/2250/8728303
dc.description.abstractEl presente escrito busca determinar el rol de distintos mecanismos cognitivos sobre la perpetuación de trampas de pobreza utilizando una muestra de estudiantes universitarios. Para esto, se realiza una revisión sobre 4 mecanismos cognitivos (desregulación emocional, carga cognitiva, autocontrol y autoeficacia [nuevo]), y se evalúa el efecto de estos mecanismos cognitivos, tanto en sus rasgos como en sus fluctuaciones de estado, sobre el logro de una tarea de valor específica: un examen parcial de los estudiantes participantes. Se encuentra que los cambios de estado en autoeficacia, autocontrol, carga cognitiva y desregulación emocional tienden a estar relacionados entre sí. También, los datos arrojan que la carga cognitiva rasgo tiene un rol importante en la explicación del nivel de logro en tareas de valor, así como el logro pasado en tareas similares y el estatus socioeconómico de los participantes. Si bien el objetivo es arrojar luces sobre las relaciones presentadas con respecto a la trampa de la pobreza, se reconoce que la aproximación aún es limitada dada la población escogida, y las técnicas de medición y análisis de datos utilizadas. Se recomienda para próximos estudios fortalecer el diseño de investigación utilizado para seguir estudiando, cada vez de manera más adecuada, el rol de los mecanismos cognitivos en la perpetuación o salida de trampas de pobreza.
dc.description.abstractThis paper seeks to identify the role of different cognitive mechanisms in the perpetuation of the poverty trap using a sample of university students. For this, a review made on 4 cognitive mechanisms (emotional dysregulation, cognitive load, self-control and self- efficacy [new]), and the effect of these cognitive mechanisms, both in their traits and in their state fluctuations, on the achievement of a specific value task is evaluated: a partial examination of the participating students. It is found that state changes in self-efficacy, self- control, cognitive load and emotional dysregulation tend to be related to each other. Also, the data show that trait cognitive load plays an important role in explaining the level of achievement in value tasks, as well as past achievement in similar tasks and the socioeconomic status of the participants. Although the objective is to shed light on the relationships presented with respect to the poverty trap, it recognizes that the approach is still limited, given: the population chosen, the measurement and data analysis techniques used. It is recommended for future studies to strengthen the research design used to continue exploring, more adequately, the role of cognitive mechanisms in the perpetuation or exit of poverty traps.
dc.languageeng
dc.publisherUniversidad de los Andes
dc.publisherPsicología
dc.publisherFacultad de Ciencias Sociales
dc.publisherDepartamento de Psicología
dc.relationAdler, N. E., Epel, E. S., Castellazzo, G., & Ickovics, J. R. (2000). Relationship of subjective and objective social status with psychological and physiological functioning: Preliminary data in healthy. White women. Health Psychology, 19(6), 586-592.
dc.relationAlkire, S., & Foster, J. (2011). "Counting and multidimensional poverty measurement." Journal of Public Economics, 95(7-8), 476-487.
dc.relationWorld Bank (2018). Poverty and Inequality. The World Bank. Retrieved from https://datatopics.worldbank.org/world-development-indicators/themes/poverty-and-inequality.html
dc.relationBaumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (1998). Ego depletion: Is the active self a limited resource? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(5), 1252-1265. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.5.1252
dc.relationBandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84(2), 191.
dc.relationBandura, A., & Wood, R. (1989). Effect of perceived controllability and performance standards on self-regulation of complex decision making. Journal of personality and social psychology, 56(5), 805.
dc.relationBanyard, V. L., & Graham-Bermann, S. A. (1998). Surviving poverty: Stress and coping in the lives of housed and homeless mothers. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 68(3), 479-489
dc.relationBaumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (2018). Ego depletion: Is the active self a limited resource? In Self-regulation and self-control (pp. 16-44). Routledge.
dc.relationBeck, Aaron T. 1967. Depression: Causes and Treatment. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.
dc.relationCannon, W. B. (1915). Bodily changes in pain, hunger, fear and rage: An account of recent researches into the function of emotional excitement. D Appleton & Company.
dc.relationBarrett, C. B., & Carter, M. R. (2013). The economics of poverty traps and persistent poverty: empirical and policy implications. The Journal of Development Studies, 49(7), 976-990.
dc.relationChen, S., Li, J., Lu, S., & Xiong, B. (2017). Escaping from poverty trap: a choice between government transfer payments and public services. Global health research and policy, 2(1), 1-16.
dc.relationClark, J., & Friesen, L. (2009). Overconfidence in forecasts of own performance: An Experimental study. The Economic Journal, 119(534), 229-251.
dc.relationClark, M. S., & Isen, A. M. (1982). Toward understanding the relationship between feeling states and social behavior. Cognitive social psychology, 73, 108.
dc.relationCroskerry, P., & Norman, G. (2008). Overconfidence in clinical decision making. The American Journal of Medicine, 121(5), S24-S29.
dc.relationCruces, G., & Wodon, Q. (2007). Risk-adjusted poverty in Argentina: measurement and determinants. The Journal of Development Studies, 43(7), 1189-1214. doi:10.1080/00220380701526329
dc.relationDel Valle, M., Galli, J. I., Urquijo, S., & Canet Juric, L. (2019). Adaptación al español de la Escala de Autocontrol y de la Escala de Autocontrol-Abreviada y evidencias de validez en población universitaria. Revista Argentina de Ciencias del Comportamiento (Argentine Journal of Behavioral Sciences), 11(2), 52-64. Retrieved from: http://www.scielo.org.ar/pdf/radcc/v11n2/1852-4206-radcc-11-02-00064.pdf
dc.relationDe Quidt, J., & Haushofer, J. (2016). Depression for economists (No. w22973). National Bureau of Economic Research.
dc.relationDale, S. (2015). Heuristics and biases. Business Information Review, 32(2), 93-99. doi:10.1177/0266382115592536
dc.relationDasgupta, D., & Saha, A. (2022). Perceptions, biases, and inequality. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 202, 198-210.
dc.relationEnglert, C., & Bertrams, A. (2021). Again, No evidence for or against the existence of ego depletion: opinion on "A multi-site preregistered paradigmatic test of the ego depletion effect". Frontiers in human neuroscience, 212.
dc.relationEvans, G. W., & English, K. (2002). The Environment of Poverty: Multiple Stressor Exposure, Psychophysiological Stress, and Socioemotional Adjustment. Child Development, 73(4), 1238-1248. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00469
dc.relationFeather, N. T. Effects of prior success and failure on expectations of success and subsequent performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1966, 3, 287-298.
dc.relationFisher, I. (1930). Theory of interest: as determined by impatience to spend income and opportunity to invest it. Augustusm Kelly Publishers, Clifton.
dc.relationFreud, A. (1974). The writings of Anna Freud: I. Introduction to psychoanalysis: Lectures for child analysts and teachers, 1922-1935. International Universities Press.
dc.relationHelion, C., & Johnson, D. D., & Fowler, J. H. (2011). The evolution of overconfidence. Nature, 477(7364), 317-320.
dc.relationGalvis, L. A., & Meisel, A. (2010). Fondo de Compensación Regional: Igualdad de oportunidades para la periferia colombiana. Documentos de trabajo sobre Economía Regional, (122), 1-43.
dc.relationGaul M (1977) Influences of anxiety level on evaluation of dimensional importance in risky tasks. Pol Psychol Bull 8:165-170
dc.relationGuiso, L., & Paiella, M. (2008). Risk aversion, wealth, and background risk. Journal of the European Economic Association, 6(6), 1109-1150.
dc.relationGuzmán-González, Mónica, Trabucco, Caterina, Urzúa M, Alfonso, Garrido, Lusmenia, & Leiva, José. (2014). Validity and reliability of the adapted Spanish version of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale in Chilean population. Psychological Therapy, 32(1), 19-29. https://dx.doi.org/10.4067/S0718-48082014000100002
dc.relationHagger, M. S., Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., Alberts, H., Anggono, C. O., Batailler, C., Birt, A. R., ... Bruyneel, S. (2016). A Multilab Preregistered Replication of the Ego-Depletion Effect. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11(4), 546-573.doi:10.1177/1745691616652873
dc.relationHaushofer, J., & Fehr, E. (2014). On the psychology of poverty. Science, 344(6186), 862-867.
dc.relationHeilman, R. M., Crian, L. G., Houser, D., Miclea, M., & Miu, A. C. (2010). Emotion regulation and decision making under risk and uncertainty. Emotion, 10(2), 257.
dc.relationHeilman, R. M., Miu, A. C., & Houser, D. (2016). Emotion Regulation and Economic Decision-Making. Studies in Neuroscience, Psychology and Behavioral Economics, 113-131.doi:10.1007/978-3-642-35923-1_7
dc.relationHelion, C., & Ochsner, K. N. (2018). The role of emotion regulation in moral judgment. Neuroethics, 11(3), 297-308.
dc.relationInzlicht, M., & Schmeichel, B. J. (2012). What is ego depletion? Toward a mechanistic revision of the resource model of self-control. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7(5), 450-463.
dc.relationJerusalem, M., & Schwarzer, R. (1992). Self-efficacy as a resource factor in stress appraisal processes. In R. Schwarzer (Ed.), Self-efficacy: Thought control of action (pp. 195-213). Washington, DC: Hemisphere.
dc.relationKahneman, D. (2012). Think fast, think slow. Debate.
dc.relationKraay, A., & McKenzie, D. (2014). Do poverty traps exist? Assessing the evidence. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 28(3), 127-48.
dc.relationLown, J. M., Kim, J., Gutter, M. S., & Hunt, A. T. (2015). Self-efficacy and savings among middle and low income households. Journal of Family and Economic Issues, 36(4), 491-502.
dc.relationMaier, S. F., & Seligman, M. E. (2016). Learned helplessness at fifty: Insights from neuroscience. Psychological review, 123(4), 349.
dc.relationMani, A., Mullainathan, S., Shafir, E., & Zhao, J. (2013). Poverty impedes cognitive function. Science, 341, 976-980.
dc.relationMartin, L. N., & Delgado, M. R. (2011). The influence of emotion regulation on decision making under risk. Journal of cognitive neuroscience, 23(9), 2569-2581.
dc.relationMischel, W. (2014). The marshmallow test: Understanding self-control and how to master it. Random House.
dc.relationMoreno, R. (2009). Cognitive load theory: more food for thought. Instructional Science, 38(2), 135-141. doi:10.1007/s11251-009-9122-9
dc.relationMuraven, M., Baumeister, R. F. (2000). Self-regulation and depletion of limited resources: Does self-control resemble a muscle? Psychological Bulletin, 126, 247-259.
dc.relationMuñoz-Martínez, A. M., Vargas, R. M., y Hoyos-González, J. S. (2016). Escala de dificultades en regulación emocional (DERS) : análisis factorial en una muestra colombiana. Acta Colombiana de Psicología, 19(1), 225-236. https://doi.org/10.14718/ACP.2016.19.1.10
dc.relationNian, M., Zheng, X., & Gao, K. (2020, December). Research on the Depletion of Psychological Resources and Poverty Circle Based on Strength Model of Self-Control. In Proceedings of the 2020 3rd International Conference on E-Business, Information Management and Computer Science (pp. 238-244).
dc.relationNunberg, H. (1942). Ego strength and ego weakness. American Imago, 3(3), 25-40.
dc.relationOtto, A.R. et al. (2013) Working-memory capacity protects model-based learning from stress. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S. A. 110, 20941-20946
dc.relationOchsner, K. N. (2018). The role of emotion regulation in moral judgment. Neuroethics, 11(3), 297-308.
dc.relationRaio, C.M. et al. (2013) Cognitive emotion regulation fails the stress test. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 110, 15139-15144
dc.relationRoss, P. H. (2019). Occupation aspirations, education investment, and cognitive outcomes: Evidence from Indian adolescents. World development, 123, 104613.
dc.relationRoy, S., Morton, M., & Bhattacharya, S. (2018). Hidden human capital: Self-efficacy, aspirations and achievements of adolescent and young women in India. World Development, 111, 161-180.
dc.relationSchilbach, F., Schofield, H., & Mullainathan, S. (2016). The psychological lives of the Poor. American Economic Review, 106(5), 435-40.
dc.relationSen, A. (1993). "The Quality of Life." Oxford University Press.
dc.relationSweller, J. (1988). Cognitive load during problem solving: Effects on learning. Cognitive science, 12(2), 257-285
dc.relationTay, L. (2023). Expiwell App (Version 2.7.8) [Mobile App]. https://apps.apple.com/co/app/expiwell/id1070733440?l
dc.relationTeasdale, J. D., & Fogarty, S. J. Differential effect of induced mood on retrieval of pleasant and unpleasant events from episodic memory. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1979, 88, 248-257
dc.relationVan der Berg, S., Burger, C., Burger, R., de Vos, M., du Rand, G., Gustafsson, M., ... & von Fintel, D. (2011). Low quality education as a poverty trap.
dc.relationVohs, K. D., & Baumeister, R. F. (Eds.). (2016). Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications. Guilford Publications.
dc.relationWang, Y. (2022). Poverty and Decision-Making: Impact and Mechanism. In 2021 International Conference on Education, Language and Art (ICELA 2021) ( pp. 26-31). Atlantis Press.
dc.relationWimmer, M. C., Dome, L., Hancock, P. J., and Wennekers, T. (2019). Is the letter cancellation task a suitable index of ego depletion? Soc. Psychol. 50, 345-354. DOI: 10.1027/1864-9335/A000393
dc.relationWuepper, D., & Lybbert, T. J. (2017). Perceived self-efficacy, poverty, and economic development. Annual Review of Resource Economics, 9(1), 383-404.
dc.relationYesuf, M., & Bluffstone, R. A. (2009). Poverty, risk aversion, and path dependence in low-income countries: Experimental evidence from Ethiopia. American Journal of Agricultural Economics, 91(4), 1022-1037.
dc.relationZimmerman, B. J. (2000). Self-efficacy: An essential motive to learn. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25(1), 82-91.
dc.rightsAttribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 Internacional
dc.rightshttp://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/
dc.rightsinfo:eu-repo/semantics/openAccess
dc.rightshttp://purl.org/coar/access_right/c_abf2
dc.titleRole of (self-efficacy and other) cognitive mechanisms in achieving valuable tasks: an approach to perpetuating poverty traps
dc.typeTrabajo de grado - Pregrado


Este ítem pertenece a la siguiente institución